3 Drawing a Research Agenda on SD and Interactional Anomie in the AC
This is what happens in the “normal” course of action, where people show “normal appearances,” behave respecting reciprocal expectations and define the situation consensually. But what happens during a global crisis such as the one we are experiencing in these months?
Goffman dealt many times with the problem of how to manage SD in circumstances (such as being in a crowded elevator) where it is not physically possible to respect socially accepted rules of conduct. And, a few times, he has dealt with emergencies. He never considered pandemics, but, for instance, in a short excerpt from his Presentation of Self in Daily Life he suggests:
Of course, at moments of great crisis, a new set of motives may suddenly become effective and the established SD between the teams may sharply increase or decrease, but when the crisis is past, the previous working consensus is likely to be re-established, albeit bashfully (Goffman, 1959, p. 197).
“Albeit bashfully” means that, in the passage from before COVID-19 (BC) to after COVID-19 (AC), we will find ourselves in a condition of, let us say, interactional anomie. By this concept I mean a condition of uncertain knowledge of what rules of conduct regarding social distance shall be applied to interactions with non-familiar people in public spaces.
In these two months of quarantine, we have been — at least partially — detrained from social interaction, we have been invited to rarefy interactions and we have been put in a condition preventing us to comply with the usual rules of conduct. The risk of contagion is in fact a new element, not yet fully normed, of social distancing. While for Goffman the so-called body idiom was mainly a matter of the symbolic expression of social differences, nowadays it has become increasingly more a vector of potential danger/risk and stigma. Further, vesting a mask, whilst playing a vital function in preventing contagion, also plays the negative effect of de-subjectifying individuals and homologating their emotional display.
In addition, we are not totally sure about what normative regulation of social interactions is nowadays valid. What will happen, soon, after the lockdown is softened? Will we approach people keeping the usual SD or will we re-frame and re-define SDs in the different ambits of our daily life? This point is particularly critical in countries such as Italy, where, together with the sense of crisis and precariousness connected to the pandemic crisis, we also experienced an overall normative uncertainty, given the contradiction between supranational, national and local norms to contain the spread of the virus.26
In the next few months, probably, we will not be very sure about which interaction order is valid and how we can properly address each other. My hypothesis is that the effects of this interactional anomie will be more evident in the situations typically forecasting an intermediate SD, such as relationships between colleagues at work, schoolmates and all those with whom we share a limited strip of our social identity. Those are, indeed, the ones in which the double contingency problem was already more significant, also in Goffman’s theory. But, if Goffman gave a moral-performative answer to the double contingency problem, he never really considered what happens when rules of conduct are not clear enough because: a) norms change and the normative framework is weak; b) the body becomes a means of danger; c) interactions are associated to fear; d) the social roles are to be redefined and so social organization, as this is the case nowadays.
Considering the interaction as an order shall also mean understanding what happens when the order changes or is in danger. This was stated by Durkheim, clearly explained by Robert K. Merton, and — I believe — needs to be integrated into Goffman’s theoretical model to make it more general and complete.
Starting from these considerations and summing up all I said so far, I propose here a research agenda on social distance to be developed in the immediate aftermath of coronavirus. Questions arising in the next months could be the following: How will keeping a given SD be interpreted by others? How will we communicate care and proximity to people if we are prevented from being physically near to them? How far will the normative quarantine produce different effects in countries that experienced different regimes of social distancing and different outcomes of the pandemic? How will we change our body idioms in encounters with partners, friends, relatives, acquaintances, colleagues and strangers? How will status dynamics be transformed in the AC? How will intimacy be reframed in the coming future?
Of course, to answer these questions properly, we need to be able to predict how long the epidemic will last, how long it will take before a vaccine is introduced and, more generally, how long it will take before we return to the previous everyday Lebenswelt, made up of rituals, face-to-face social relations and interactions in crowded collective spaces. We need also to be able to predict how accelerated will be the technological turn towards the dematerialization of work, the digitalization of social relationships, the transformation of social control and interaction at a distance in educational contexts (Rosa, 2013).
By schematizing this reasoning, one can expect the following processes to occur in the immediate aftermath of this first COVID-19 crisis:
Increasing digitalization of proximity rituals;
Increasing transformation of work interactions into smart-working interactions;
Increasing SD and interactional anomie in contexts such as school, work, leisure time;
Increasing SD in contexts of civil inattention, such as sidewalks, public places, public transport, commercial areas;
Increasing stigmatization of already stigmatized groups, such as former prisoners, foreigners, refugees;
Increasing subrogation of social proximity rituals through indirect forms of proximity (verbalisations, “secondary adjustments,” i.e. forms of resistance to the rules of institutional conduct) and new forms of positive proximity (e.g. neighborhood relations, mutualistic solidarity within areas of cohabitation).
Some of those hypothesized processes are transformations of off-line into on-line interactions, or, to be more precise, they move the already existing continuity between online and offline interactions toward the first pole. John B. Thompson, who applied the Goffmanian perspective to media and modernity in his well-known contribution (1995) has recently proposed to introduce a new theoretical category to indicate this kind of interactions. He spoke of mediated online interaction to indicate a new form of communication who has the four following features: a) it is stretched out in time and space; b) it has a narrowing range of symbolic clues;27 c) it is dialogical; d) it is oriented from many to many (Thompson, 2020). SD is included in different degrees in all the four points. It regards how we use metaphorically space and time; how we use symbolic clues to express proximity or distance; how we engage ourselves in a dialog; how we keep personal, interpersonal or socially oriented distance.28
5 (Albeit Bashful) Conclusions
In brief, this article, starting from Goffman’s perspective, has shown how the concept of social distance is far more complex than the one used in the official communication of international health agencies. It is not simply a dimension concerning physical distance between people; rather it is an element in the “grammar” of interactional order. It is a complex matter including moral, ritual, organizational and functional aspects.
All these dimensions in Western societies tend to remain stable and to be implemented through behavioral obligations and expectations. As a result of globalization, the Western model of social distancing has spread throughout the world. All this makes Goffman’s theories even more salient today. The COVID-19 pandemic inserts a new and unexpected element in the regulation of social interactions: the dangers of contamination and contagion inherent in interpersonal relationships. All this, in my hypothesis, will lead to a more or less extended phase of interactional anomie in which people will find it difficult to recognize what rule of conduct regulates a changed interactional order. In Goffman’s terms, probably, this will lead to a regulatory looseness and to an extended need for working consensus between interlocutors each time we encounter someone in an uncertain role relationship with us. In my terms, interactional anomie shall be considered as a pragmatic and processual step in the process of reorganizing a changing interaction order.
This article is not based on solid empirical data but constitutes, as anticipated, above all an exercise in sociological imagination.32 Such imagination aims more to open up a research agenda than to advance detailed forecasts about the future.
References
Bogardus, E.S. (1925). Measuring Social Distances. Journal of Applied Sociology, 9: 299–308.
Burns, T. (2002). Erving Goffman. London: Routledge.
Couldry, N., & Hepp, A. (2016). The Mediated Construction of Reality. London: Wiley.
Elias, N. (2001). Society of Individuals. New York: Bloomsbury Publishing.
Goffman, E. (1952). On Cooling the Mark Out: Some Aspects of Adaptation to Failure. Psychiatry, 15(4), 451–463. https://doi.org/10.1080/00332747.1952.11022896
Goffman, E. (1955). On Face-Work: An Analysis of Ritual Elements in Social Interaction. Psychiatry, 18(3), 213–231. https://doi.org/10.1080/00332747.1955.11023008
Goffman, E. (1956). The Nature of Deference and Demeanor. American Anthropologist, 58(3), 473–502. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1956.58.3.02a00070
Goffman, E. (1959). The Presentation of Self in Daily Life. New York: Doubleday.
Goffman, E. (1961). Asylums: Essays on the Social Situation of Mental Patients and Other Inmates. Garden City, NY: Anchor Books, Doubleday & Co.
Goffman, E. (1963a). Behavior in Public Places: Notes on the Social Organization of Gatherings. New York: The Free Press.
Goffman, E. (1963b). Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled Identity. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.
Goffman, E. (1967). Interaction Ritual: Essays on Face-to-Face Behavior. New York: Anchor Books.
Goffman, E. (1969). Strategic Interaction. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania.
Goffman, E. (1971). Relations in Public: Microstudies of the Public Order. New York: Basic Books.
Goffman, E. (1981). Forms of Talk. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Goffman, E. (1983). The Interaction Order. American Sociological Review, 48(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.2307/2095141
Lofland, J. (1969). Deviance and Identity. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Lofland, J. (1980). Early Goffman: Style, Structure, Substance, Soul. In J. Ditton (ed.), The View from Goffman (pp. 24–51). London: Palgrave Macmillan.
Manning, P.K. (1976). The Decline of Civility: A Comment on Erving Goffman’s Sociology. Canadian Review of Sociology and Anthropology, 13, 13–25. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1755-618X.1976.tb00755.x
Parsons, T. (1951). The Social System. New York: Free Press.
Rosa, H. (2013). Social Acceleration: A New Theory of Modernity. New York: Columbia University Press.
Simmel, G. (1949). The Sociology of Sociability. American Journal of Sociology, 55(3), 254–261. https://doi.org/10.1086/220534
Simmel, G. (2018). On a Psychology of Shame. Digithum, 21, 67–74. (Original work published 1911). http://doi.org/10.7238/d.v0i21.3116
Thompson, J.B. (1995). The Media and Modernity: A Social Theory of the Media. Cambridge: Polity; Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Thompson, J.B. (2020). Mediated Interaction in the Digital Age. Theory, Culture and Society, 37(1), 3–28. https://doi.org/10.1177/0263276418808592
Turner, R.H. (1990). Role Change. Annual Review of Sociology, 16(1), 87–110. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.so.16.080190.000511
Vanderstraeten, R. (2002). Parsons, Luhmann and the Theorem of Double Contingency. Journal of Classical Sociology, 2(1), 77–92. https://doi.org/10.1177/1468795X02002001684
Verhoeven, J.C. (1993). An Interview with Erving Goffman. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 26(3), 317–348. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327973rlsi2603_5
Wright Mills, C. (1959). The Sociological Imagination. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/prevent-getting-sick/social-distancing.html. Retrieved April 18, 2020.↩︎
European Center for Disease Control and Prevention. https://www.ecdc.europa.eu/en/publications-data/considerations-relating-social-distancing-measures-response-covid-19-second. Retrieved April 18 2020.↩︎
WHO, “Coronavirus Press Conference,” March 20, 2020. The full transcription of the meeting is available at: https://www.who.int/docs/default-source/coronaviruse/transcripts/who-audio-emergencies-coronavirus-press-conference-full-20mar2020.pdf?sfvrsn=1eafbff_0↩︎
Ibidem, p. 6.↩︎
Harmeet Kaur, Forget “social distancing.” The WHO prefers we call it “physical distancing” because social connections are more important than ever, CNN. Available at: https://edition.cnn.com/2020/04/15/world/social-distancing-language-change-trnd/index.html↩︎
Despite being the most widely read sociology book (Burns, 2002), a survey conducted by the International Sociological Association in 1997 found The Presentation of Self in Daily Life to be only the 10th most influential book in the career of sociologists, accounting for only 5.5% of total respondents.↩︎
“It is a widely held notion that restrictions placed upon contact, the maintenance of social distance, provide a way in which awe can be generated and sustained in the audience a way, as Kenneth Burke has said, in which the audience can be held in a state of mystification in regard to the performer.” (Goffman, 1959, p. 67).↩︎
“A rule of conduct may be defined as a guide for action, recommended not because it is pleasant, cheap, or effective, but because it is suitable or just. Infractions characteristically lead to feelings of uneasiness and to negative social sanction (…) Rules of conduct impinge upon the individual in two general ways: directly, as obligations, establishing how he is morally constrained to conduct himself; indirectly, as expectations, establishing how others are morally bound to act in regard to him” (Goffman, 1956, pp. 473–474).↩︎
“During informal social intercourse it is well understood that an effort on the part of one person (ego) to decrease his social distance from another person (alter) must be graciously accepted by alter or, if rejected, rejected tactfully so that the initiator of the move can save his social face. This rule is codified in books on etiquette and is followed in actual behavior. A friendly movement in the direction of alter is a movement outward on a limb; ego communicates his belief that he has defined himself as worthy of alter’s society, while at the same time he places alter in the strategic position of being able to discredit this conception” (Goffman, 1952, p. 456).↩︎
“The term face may be denied as the positive social value a person effectively claims for himself by the line others assume he has taken during a particular contact. Face is an image of self-delineated in terms of approved social attributes — albeit an image that others may share, as when a person makes a good showing for his profession or religion by making a good showing for himself” (Goffman, 1967, p. 5)↩︎
“Of course, in the matter of keeping social distance, the audience itself will often co-operate by acting in a respectful fashion, in awed regard for the sacred integrity imputed to the performer.” (Goffman, 1959, p. 67).↩︎
“Embarrassment has to do with unfulfilled expectations but not of a statistical kind. Given their social identities and the setting, the participants will sense what sort of conduct ought to be maintained as the appropriate thing, however much they may despair of its actually occurring. An individual may firmly expect that certain others will make him ill at ease, and yet this knowledge may increase his discomfiture instead of lessening it. An entirely unexpected flash of social engineering may save a situation, all the more effectively for being unanticipated. The expectations relevant to embarrassment are moral, then, but embarrassment does not arise from the breach of any moral expectation, for some infractions give rise to resolute moral indignation and no uneasiness at all.” (Goffman, 1956, p. 268).↩︎
In this sense, Goffman’s sociology recalls both the late Durkheimian interpretation of Kant’s pure reason and the pragmatist understanding of Kantian practical reason as attention to alter’s standpoint, offered by both John Dewey and George H. Mead.↩︎
As one can read in the Preface to The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life, Goffman considers “the way in which the individual in ordinary work situations presents himself and his activity to others, the ways in which he guides and controls the impression they form of him, and the kinds of things he may and may not do while sustaining his performance before them” (Goffman, 1959, p. XI).↩︎
“In dealing with rules of conduct it is convenient to distinguish two classes, symmetrical and asymmetrical (…). A symmetrical rule is one which leads an individual to have obligations or expectations regarding others that these others have in regard to him (…). An asymmetrical rule is one that leads others to treat and be treated by an individual differently from the way he treats and is treated by them.” (1956, p. 476).↩︎
In an interview with Verhoeven, Goffman said: “The way I differ from social constructionists is that I don’t think the individual himself or herself does much of the constructing. He rather comes to a world, already in some sense or other, established. So, there I would differ from persons who use in their writing the notion of social construction of reality” (Verhoeven, 1993, p. 320). This topic is important for our discussion, because it shows how a high normativity on one’s behaviour could produce an high interactional anomie in times of normative confusion and fast social change.↩︎
“A noteworthy individual often produces in passers-by a standard ‘behaviour sequence’ during which he is stared at until close enough for an exchange of recognition signals, then civil inattention is proffered until he is just beyond the sight line, whereupon the curious turn and stare once again. The implication is that whereas direct staring is to be avoided, one is free to be exposed in one’s staring before those whom one is not staring at.” (Goffman, 1971, p. 126).↩︎
“What seems to be involved is that one gives to another enough visual notice to demonstrate that one appreciates that the other is present (and that one admits openly to having seen him), while at the next moment withdrawing one’s attention from him so as to express that he does not constitute a target of special curiosity or desire” (Goffman, 1963b, p. 84).↩︎
For double contingency we refer here to the classic concept introduced by Parsons (1951) relating to the hazard present in any encounter, given the fact that both participants’ behaviour is, at least partially, contingent.↩︎
Again, we find here a neo-Kantian element: the minimal conditions of social stability.↩︎
This subject deserves a wider development. For reasons of space and relevance, we will not do so in this article. For an extended review see Vanderstraeten, 2002.↩︎
See Turner, 1990.↩︎
“A basic social arrangement in modem society is that the individual tends to sleep, play, and work· in different places, with different co-participants, under different authorities, and without an over-all rational plan. The central feature of total institutions can be described as a breakdown of the barriers ordinarily separating these three spheres of life” (Goffman, 1961, pp. 5–6).↩︎
Especially in the second part of his career, this integration will lead him to develop a greater closeness to ethnomethodology.↩︎
On an existentialist reading of Goffman, much has been written. See, among others, Manning (1976) and Lofland (1980).↩︎
This is the case of what happened in Italy during the lockdown, which started on March 3, 2020 and will be extended, in very restrictive forms, until May 4. It has been calculated that during the 100 days of quarantine 763 different acts have been introduced by national, regional and local authorities (source: IlSole24Ore, “In cento giorni di lotta al virus 763 atti di Governo e Regioni,” May 6, 2020). In this time range, the Italian Government has produced a normative regulation of the lockdown almost entirely based on emergency decrees. Especially in March, the national regulation of the lockdown changed very quickly, from week to week, thus leading to a substantial normative instability. A further anomalous situation, produced by regional and municipal authorities, was added to this because of local regulations that were heterogeneous and often contradictory to national decrees. This meant that in the very restricted interactions allowed outside the housing context, citizens were faced with contradictory indications with respect to the maximum permitted distance of movement, the obligation to wear gloves and masks in the street or only in shops and the possibility to exchange conversation with a neighbor or a friend met on the street. A further source of anomie are the numerous cases of disparity in the application of sanctions implemented by the various police forces in the territory.↩︎
An interesting recent example is the introduction of the care reaction in Facebook.↩︎
Again, this is a subject deserving a wider development. For a recent and focussed review see Couldry & Hepp, 2016.↩︎
E. Koeze and N. Popper. “The virus changed the way we internet.” The New York Times, April 7, 2020.↩︎
Anonymous student, “Love in the time of Coronavirus.” Social Media class, University of Roskilde, 2020. This content is published and anonymized thanks to the consent of the author.↩︎
Gabriele Battaglia, “Il distanziamento sociale in Cina è la nuova normalità.” Internazionale. Available at: https://www.internazionale.it/reportage/gabriele-battaglia/2020/04/17/cina-distanziamento-sociale-normalita?fbclid=IwAR2IWc7cYeeZEqMjYdp23-YA1huaTUGekcpqE_7ql2P4VV9pDjTFK2f6gGI↩︎
Charles Wright Mills defined sociological imagination as “the awareness of the relationship between personal experience and the wider society” (Mills, 1959, p. 5). This is what I tried to do here, helping my self with the stimulation of the imaginative background of Goffman’s theory.↩︎
1 Social Distance or Physical Distance? How International Health Agencies Communicate
Why is it interesting to focus on the sociological aspects of social distance? Because, I believe, it is a far more complex concept in terms of both its use of common sense and the typical use of institutional rhetoric. Let us start with the latter, considering briefly the official documents about the policies to contain and stop contagion from COVID-19.
Since February 2020, in the documents published by international health agencies, the expression social distance has been mainly used as a synonym of physical distance. Following is an example coming from the United States Centers for Disease Control and Prevention:
In addition to the synonymic use, what is most interesting in the document is the implicit equation of sociality with risk and the semantic opposition between health and sociability: salus vs societas. Avoiding social contacts means avoiding potential contagion. Interaction = danger. In fact, to different degrees and in different ways, during the lockdown period all countries limited all purely “social” interactions, that is, all ludic and non-instrumental interactions, in a Simmelian sense (Simmel, 1949).
In very similar terms, the European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control suggests on its website:
Finally, the World Health Organization (WHO), in the first weeks of the crisis, called on citizens all over the world to maintain as much as possible a “social distance” from others in order to prevent contagion. But since the press conference3 held on March 20, 2020, the policy has changed. Since then, WHO has preferred to use the expression “physical distance” to refer to the recommended measures of movements restriction in order to avoid the negative effects of a message that could push people into social isolation and alienation. As Dr Maria Van Kerkhove suggested:
The shift has led to some skepticism in the scientific community. This is what two social scientists reported to CNN about the case5:
In any case, the shift in WHO communication did not much affect the global use of the terms on the Internet. Analyzing Google queries, what results is that the curve of social distance relevance is largely more significantly associated with the virus spread than the curve of physical distance (see Fig. 1).
The reported scientific controversy about the use of the two expressions (“social distance” vs “physical distance”) is interesting as it demonstrates the possible effects of a medicalization of social relationships. Compared to sociological conceptions, in the health agencies’ view the performative expression of social distance is limited to physical distance. But those agencies also suggest — explicitly or implicitly — the opportunity of limiting almost any “unnecessary” interaction with strangers, at least until the virus will be defeated.